By René A. Azeez, Honours BSc. Developmental Biology, University of Toronto. April 21, 2011.
Have you ever wondered why we make the decisions that we make? Human nature is, on the face of it, determined by a combination of cognitive and emotional processes. That is not to say that human beings can be generalized into percentages of rationality and emotionality, but what determines this ratio is a fascinating question and has been explored by researchers.
Interestingly, it had been previously shown that in a scenario known as the ultimatum game, in which there exists a “proposer” and a “responder” that may share a sum of money as suggested by the proposer, human nature as the responder is to reject all offers that permit the proposer more than 80% of the total sum. Rational thought would advocate that the responder accept all non-zero offers but humans tend to prefer to gain nothing than to gain what they consider to be an unfair portion. Brain imaging confirms that rejection of the ultimatum game correspond to activation of portions of the brain correlated with emotions. Why are some of us more capable of favoring rational thought? Who is capable of uncoupling emotional reactions from actual behavior and what makes them so?
New Research published in the April edition of Frontiers in Decision Neuroscience by Ulrich Kirk, Human Neuroimaging Laboratory at Virginia Tech; Jonathan Downar, University of Toronto; and Read Montague, Virginia Tech; shows that Buddhist meditators are far more likely to take what is offered.
Mindfulness meditation advocates a spirit of observation as opposed to judgment and involves techniques developed with the goal of well-being and emotional balance. This lead the researchers to believe that meditators would be more accepting in the ultimatum game of rewards that were socially considered unfair, than non-mediators. What they found was that the areas of the brain normally activated by an unfair proposal, specifically the anterior insula which is associated with emotions of disgust, violations, rejection and betrayal; were as expected, expressed in the non-meditator control group. However, meditators showed “no significant activation for either fair or unfair offers, and there was no significant relationship between anterior insula and offer rejection”.
Meditators are therefore able to uncouple the negative emotional social understanding of an unfair offer from their decision making process. The western world has only recently begun to embrace mindfulness meditation but the origins of mindfulness meditation date back 2500 years in Buddhist tradition. Meditators were found to draw on areas of the brain involved with attending to internal bodily states and attention to the present moment, whereas the control group of non-meditators drew upon areas associated with episodic memory and fictive error.
They concluded that “the trick may lie not in rational calculation, but in steering away from what-if scenarios, and concentrating on the interoceptive qualities that accompany any reward, no matter how small.”
REFERENCE: Kirk U, Downar J and Montague P. Interoception drives increased rational decision-making in meditators playing the Ultimatum Game. Front. Neurosci., 5:49; 2011.